Mystery of ocean garbage partially solved, but questions remain

Mystery of ocean garbage partially solved, but questions remain
Share this
Share

A significant amount of the world’s plastic ends up in our oceans, which has caused something of a mystery for ecologists, who until recently had found evidence of only tens of thousands of tons of that garbage rather than the millions they expected would be floating on the surface. Now a joint-team of researchers has found evidence that plastic particles have been accumulating in deep sea sediment for the past century. But some questions remain–namely the how of the problem. Lead researcher Dr Lucy Woodall points to various methods by which the plastic is making its way to the bottom–methods which reflect the specific nature of the waters in which garbage is discarded.

The team–composed of researchers from the Natural History Museum of London, the Scottish Marine Institute, and the universities of Barcelona, Oxford, and Plymouth– looked at samples of from 12 sites over three bodies of water: the Atlantic, the Indian, and Mediterranean. The samples were collected between 2001 and 2012 at depths of 300-3,000 meters, and contained high levels of plastic garbage particles–called “microplastics” for their size of less than 1mm.

While the surface of the ocean is “mysteriously” clean of garbage–only around 35,000 of the millions of tons of plastic garbage currently in our oceans are expected to still be at the surface–the deposits in the ocean’s sediment were mixed with 1,000 times that amount of plastic.

Read more: Ocean Garbage Mystery: Instead of Expected Millions of Tons, Researchers Find Only 7,000 – 35,000 Tons

The finding by the joint team marks an important step in understanding how plastics make their way through ocean ecosystems. The mystery remains, however, as to how the plastics make their way from the surface to the sediment.

Mystery of ocean garbage partially solved, but questions remain
Dr Lucy Woodall

Lead researcher on the study, Dr Lucy Woodall of the Department of Life Sciences at the Natural History Museum in London, told The Speaker that the team also has a pretty good idea of the various processes by which the plastic makes its way to the bottom

“We speculate that similar oceanographic mechanisms as act on plastic fibers act on other particles, such as dense shelf cascading, severe coastal storms, offshore convection and saline subduction,” Woodall told us. “We suggest additionally that ‘Colonization by organisms, adherence to phytoplankton and the aggregation with organic debris and small particles in the form of marine snow will eventually enhance settling.’

“Specific topography of the deep sea will also carry with it specific processes, for example submarine canyons are known to act as conduits to deep areas, and taylor columns–specific currents–over seamounts could result in retention of plastics at these sites.”

Although the total garbage accumulated in the earth’s oceans are calculated at hundreds of thousands to millions of tons, this is only a fraction of the total plastic produced and discarded. A recent study found that the amount of garbage in the ocean is only around 0.1 percent of the amount we produce each year.

“Further studies specifically addressing the process of plastics moving to depth are required so that we can begin to understand the impact of these pollutants in the environment,” said Woodall.

The report, “The deep sea is a major sink for microplastic debris,” was completed by Lucy C. Woodall, Anna Sanchez-Vidal, Miquel Canals, Gordon L.J. Paterson, Rachel Coppock, Victoria Sleight, Antonio Calafat, Alex D. Rogers, Bhavani E. Narayanaswamy, and Richard C. Thompson, and was published in the journal Royal Society Open Science this week.

By James Haleavy

Adding water to solids can actually make them stronger, providing engineers with exciting new material composites – Yale research

Adding water to solids can actually make them stronger, providing engineers with exciting new material composites - Yale research
Share this
Share

Contrary to intuition, adding pockets of water to solids can actually make them stronger. This finding, the result of research by Yale scientists, offers “a new knob to turn” for engineers, the researchers say. Engineers will be able to add exciting new properties to composite materials–such as electromagnetism–by embedding droplets of liquid, and, on a purely scientific level, the research provides valuable insight into the nature of the material properties at small and large scales–how the relative strengths of a material at one size can be opposite to that at another size.

“This is a great example of how different types of physics emerge at different scales,” Dr. Eric Dufresne, associate professor of mechanical engineering and materials science at Yale and principle investigator of the study, told The Speaker. “Shrinking the scale of an object can really change how it behaves.”

Adding water to solids can actually make them stronger, providing engineers with exciting new material composites - Yale research (5)
Dr. Eric Dufresne

Usually, replacing parts of a solid with liquid–generally considered a soft material–makes the material weaker, but this is not always the case. The researchers strengthened solids with liquids by the virtue of the surface tension of liquid droplets.

“Surface tension is a force that tries to reduce the surface area of a material,” Dufresne told us. “It is familiar in fluids–it’s the force that pulls water into a sponge, makes wet hair clump together and lets insects walk on water. Solids have surface tension too, but usually the ‘elastic force’ of the solid is so strong that surface tension doesn’t have much of an effect. The ‘elastic force’ of a solid is what makes a solid spring back to its originial shape after you stop pushing on it.

Adding water to solids can actually make them stronger, providing engineers with exciting new material composites - Yale research (4)
Stretching droplets embedded in soft solids. The sample is clamped and stretched in the x-direction.

Because the tendency of a liquid is to have as small a surface as possible, embedding small drops of liquid–about a micron in diameter–strengthen solids because the surface tension of the water provides stiffness to the composite.

Dufresne commented on what would be, in his words, “a new knob to turn” for engineers, who can achieve greater control over the properties of composite materials by including fluids.

Adding water to solids can actually make them stronger, providing engineers with exciting new material composites - Yale research (4)
Example images of ionic-liquid droplets in a soft, silicone solid E = 1.7kPa. Larger droplets deform more at the same applied strain. Overlay shows small (blue), medium (red) and large (green) droplet images combined together for shape comparison.

“As the solid gets stiffer, the liquid droplets need to be smaller in order to have this stiffening or cloaking effect. By embedding the solid with droplets of different materials, one can give it new electrical, optical or mechanical properties.

“On the simple scale, they could lower the cost be replacing expensive polymers with simple liquids. More excitingly, embedded droplets could provide an electromagnetic handle to actuate structures.”

In the recent research, the team embedded the small drops of liquid into silicone and then stretched the silicone. Silicone embedded with large drops of water deformed easily–the material was weakened by the liquid. Silicone with small droplets resisted deformation–the material was strengthened.

Adding water to solids can actually make them stronger, providing engineers with exciting new material composites - Yale research (4)
Aspect ratio of stretched ionic-liquid droplets in a soft silicone gel as a function of size and strain. Different colors correspond to different applied strains.

The team found that a composite up to 30 percent stronger than pure silicone could be created by embedding a large amount of small liquid droplets.

Dufresne explained how the current work came about.

“A few years back, we discovered, on accident, some surprises on how liquid droplets sit on top of solid surfaces,” said Dufresne. “In the course of that work, we realized people needed to pay attention to solid surface tension. Since then, we have been looking for other examples where solid surface tension might be an important and neglected component of the behaviour of materials. These experiments were inspired by ongoing efforts in ‘metamaterials’ where engineers tune the microstructure of a material to give it new properties.”

“It turns out that the importance of surface tension is inversely proportional to the size,” Dufresne said of the study. “So what’s just a negligible force for big things becomes a strong force for very small things–which in turn can strongly affect the material as a whole.”

The report, “Stiffening solids with liquid inclusions,” was completed by Drs. W. Style, Rostislav Boltyanskiy, Benjamin Allen, Katharine E.Jensen, Henry P. Foote, John S. Wettlaufer, and Eric R. Dufresne, and was publishe in December’s Nature Physics.

Photos: all belong to the work of the Yale team

Women over three times as empathetic to their partners as men

Women over three times as empathetic to their partners as men
Share this
Share

Women feel what is happening to their partners over three times as much as men do. According to new research, the difference between the empathy felt by women and men was the biggest of many factors analyzed.

“In our work, we were trying to measure how partners affect each other’s mental health through life events,” Dr Cindy Mervin, research fellow at Griffith University’s Centre for Applied Health Economics and lead author of the study, told The Speaker. “[O]ur work showed that negative and positive things that happen to individuals not only affect them but also affect their family.”

Women over three times as empathetic to their partners as men
Dr Cindy Mervin

Mervin explained the research team’s findings about the levels of empathy felt by women and men, most notably, that women’s levels of empathy for their partners–at 24 percent of what they would have felt had an event happened to themselves–are over 300 percent of men’s levels.

“We can interpret the 24 percent by saying that on average women will be affected by the events happening to their partner by about 24 percent of the degree to which they are affected by their own,” Mervin told us. “In other words, women are affected about four times as much by the events happening to them than events happening to their partners.”

The research involved questionnaire data from the Australian study Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) on over 20,000 people across the country. The team looked at partners in both straight and same-sex relationships who did not separate during the observed panel, which amounted to just under 11,000 individuals and over 53,000 person-year observations.

The research led the team to conclude that while women’s empathy toward their partners was the strongest found in their study, men on average were found to not be empathetic in any significant way.

“We estimated this coefficient for different types of respondents–women vs men, parents vs their counterparts, and individual from high-income households vs. those from low-income households,” stated Mervin. “The highest value we found was for women when compared to men. For men, we found a value around seven percent and therefore found that men were not significantly affected by things happening to their partner.”

Mervin clarified that the findings do not mean that men are unemotional or uncaring, but that their care does not extend to their partner the way women’s care does.

“Although the degree measured for women and men is different, it does not mean that men are unemotional as they are quite strongly affected by what happens to themselves,” said Mervin. “They are just not very emotional when it comes to their feelings of their partner.”

The report, “Is shared misery double misery,” was authored by Drs Merehau Cindy Mervin and Paul Frigters of University of Queensland’s School of Economics, and was published in the journal Social Science and Medicine.

By James Haleavy

Nobel physicist thinks “theory of everything” will be found at a million billionths

Nobel physicist thinks theory of everything will be found at a million billionths
Share this
Share

Ten-17 and 10-19 of the radius of an atomic nucleus, or a million billionths of that nucleus. That is the range at which he expects really new physics to be found, and that is where we will find the “theory of everything,” Nobel-prize winning theoretical physicist Steven Weinberg said at Harvard’s Geological Hall this week.

The unification of the four known forces of nature can be found at what Weinberg said was a “crude estimate”–a ballpark range at which “it all seems to hang together.”

“The big question that we face… is, can we find a truly fundamental theory uniting all the forces, including gravitation… characterized by tiny lengths like 10-17 to 10-19 nuclear radii?” said Weinberg, as reported by the Harvard Gazette. “Is it a string theory? That seems like the most beautiful candidate, but we don’t have any direct evidence that it is a string theory. The only handle we have… on this to do further experiments is in cosmology.”

Weinberg referred to the level at which the four forces could be explained in his speech. The two problems that physicists consider to be the less familiar of the four fundamental forces of nature are atomic in level. One force holds a nucleus together. The other is responsible for radioactive decay, changing one particle to another. No theory exists that would explain how all four forces work. The theory of gravity explains one of the better understood of the four forces. Another theory describes the subatomic interactions of electromagnetism and strong and weak nuclear interactions.

Weinberg referred to the far extremes of tininess where the strong and weak forces converge. The strong force weakens at shorter scales and the other two nuclear interactions get stronger at the same scale, apparently.

Gravity, Weinberg said, was strongly suggested to be unified somehow with the other three forces at that same scale, because of the required mass for two protons or electrons to balance their repulsive electrical force.

However, that range is a challenging area for physicists. Required for that kind of investigation is technology beyond what scientists currently have–10 trillion times what we can currently offer physicists in terms of energy is needed, Weinberg said.

Steven Weinberg won the Nobel Prize in 1979 for his work on electroweak theory with Sheldon Glashow and Abdus Salam, in which the relationship between the weak force and magnetism was explained.

By James Haleavy

Greenland’s ice sheets melting faster than we know

Greenland's ice sheets melting faster than we know
Share this
Share

If global warming is not a concern for many yet, the fast pace at which the icy landmass of Greenland is melting should be something to think about. New research has suggested that Greenland is melting at a pace quicker than the one earlier models predicted.

According to new research by the National Snow and Ice Data Center, it is estimated that if Greenland’s ice sheet melted, sea level would rise six meters (or 20 feet), causing grave threats such as floods and storms, and displace millions as it happened.

In the 20th century alone, the sea level rose by 6.7 inches and it is estimated that this number will only increase, roughly by two to five times–an alarming 11 to 37 inches–by the turn of the 22nd century, according to the latest report by Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC).

To prove their point, scientists have collected data from NASA’s ICESat spacecraft and Operation IceBridge, plotting the elevation of 100,000 sites on Greenland from 1993 to 2012. According to the research, Greenland has already lost an average of 243 gigatons of ice during the period between 2003 and 2009, adding 0.027 inches to the sea level per year.

Post-analysis, researchers were able to make estimations on the amount of ice that has melted over the years. A quite intricate melting pattern in itself, scientists believe that although they cannot conclusively predict the amount of ice that will melt in Greenland in the future, the data does underline the degree of dilemma we face with regards to glacial melting now.

Beata Csatho, a study author and professor of Geology at the University at Buffalo said of past findings, “My personal opinion is that most of the predictions of this as far as Greenland is concerned are too low.”

Csatho and her colleagues found the complex pattern hard to decipher and interpret in layman’s terms because a drop in the temperature thickens the ice, instead of melting it.

To understand the research better, scientists divided Greenland’s glaciers into seven groups based on characteristics of their melting pattern from 2003 to 2009. “Understanding the groupings will help us pick out examples of glaciers that are representative of the whole. We can then use data from these representative glaciers in models to provide a more complete picture of what is happening.” Csatho concluded.

The researcher pointed out that the currently-used model ignores the extensive ice loss in southeastern Greenland, which, after all, contributes to more than half of the ice loss in Greenland. Another issue the research noted was that current predictions are based on data is being from Kangerlussuaq, Petermann, Helheim, and Jakobshavn, a team that measured only four of Greenland’s 242 glaciers. The other 238 glaciers exhibit different behaviours throughout the year, according to the researchers–a fact that has been ignored and probably contributed to the recently discovery of a rate of melting that is quite different from what was thought before.

Analysis by Rathan Paul Harshavardan

Image: NASA

Alcohol interferes with sleep, but not by disrupting the circadian clock

Share this
Share

A drink before bed? Around 50 million adults in the US take a drink to help them fall asleep, but that drink is being advised against by researchers at the University of Missouri School of Medicine. Their research has found that alcohol actually disrupts sleep–even if it causes people to nod off. It disrupts sleep differently from what is commonly believed, however. Rather than the circadian rhythm, alcohol actually affects the body’s homeostatic system.

“Based on our results, it’s clear that alcohol should not be used as a sleep aid,” Dr. Pradeep Sahota, chair of the MU School of Medicine’s Department of Neurology and an author of the study, said in the press release.

Around one-third of our people’s lives are spent sleeping, and around 20 percent of America’s 250 million adults use alcohol. This is relevant to the nation’s health and economy, the researchers pointed out. They cited research that has found alcohol-related sleep disorders cost the US  at least $18 billion per year.

The MU team have spent five years studying the interaction between sleep and alcohol, and have concluded in their most recent report that alcohol disturbs sleep, but in a way that may surprise scientists and readers alike.

“The prevailing thought was that alcohol promotes sleep by changing a person’s circadian rhythm–the body’s built-in 24-hour clock,” said Dr Mahesh Thakkar, another author of the study. “However, we discovered that alcohol actually promotes sleep by affecting a person’s sleep homeostasis–the brain’s built-in mechanism that regulates your sleepiness and wakefulness.”

The body has two systems that both play a role in sleep. The homeostatic system builds up pressure to sleep the longer a person stays awake, and the circadian system is an internal clock regulated by the body’s perception of light and dark.

A person might drink a lot of coffee and power through a night without sleeping. In the morning they would feel increased pressure to sleep from their homeostatic system, but their circadian clock would tell them it was time to be awake.

Alcohol does something of the opposite, the researchers found. It promotes sleepiness through the homeostatic system while leaving the circadian rhythm unaffected. While a person may nod off more quickly, it will be likely that they will not sleep through the whole night.

“Alcohol disrupts sleep and the quality of sleep is diminished. Additionally, alcohol is a diuretic, which increases your need to go the bathroom and causes you to wake up earlier in the morning,” Sahota pointed out.

The researchers advised other options that could be pursued by people having difficulty getting a good night’s rest.

“If you are experiencing difficulty sleeping, don’t use alcohol,”said Thakkar. “Talk to your doctor or a sleep medicine physician to determine what factors are keeping you from sleeping. These factors can then be addressed with individualized treatments.”

The report, “Alcohol Disrupts Sleep Homeostasis,” was completed by Drs Mahesh Thakkar, Pradeep Sahota and Rishi Sharma, and was published in the international biomedical journal Alcohol.

By Cheryl Bretton

 

 

 

 

Even a single mine can damage fish habitats miles downstream – study

“Mines have a much stronger influence on fishes than has been assumed," Dr. Wesley Daniel, a research associate at MSU and lead author of the study, said. "It’s important, when considering the location of a new mine, to not just look local--but look way downstream.” Here Dr Daniel explains the research, which has relevance in every part of the world where mining occurs.
Share this
Share

A unique study of landscape factors–in particular, mines–as regional sources of stress has been conducted by Michigan State University researchers. The study involved waterways in 33 U.S. states and 22,000 fish community samples. The researchers were themselves surprised by the results: even a single mine can damage fish habitats in larger rivers downstream, and the effects can reach streams not even directly connected to a mine. 

“Mines have a much stronger influence on fishes than has been assumed,” Dr. Wesley Daniel, a research associate at MSU and lead author of the study, said. “It’s important, when considering the location of a new mine, to not just look local–but look way downstream.”

Here Dr Daniel explains the research, which has relevance in every part of the world where mining occurs.


 

Even a single mine can damage fish habitats miles downstream - study
Dr. Wesley Daniel

Our study was funded by the US Fish and Wildlife and US Geological Survey as part of efforts to characterize associations between landscape factors, including coal and mineral mines, on stream fish communities in a range of stream sizes in the eastern US.

We wanted to test whether mines operate as a regional source of stress to fish communities over large spatial extents, as has been shown in many previous works examining urban and agricultural land use.

One of the striking results was the clear and consistent negative associations between fish and mines across all three regions, and that these relationships held true for many different groupings of fishes. Examples of groupings include game species (fish species targeted by anglers like trout, bass, catfish), intolerant species that cannot endure much disturbance, fishes that use various habitats for spawning or their life cycle, and tolerant species that are often found to increase in disturbed areas. Even a single mine can damage fish habitats miles downstream - study (4)We found that tolerant species decreased in abundance with increased mine density in the watershed.

We found that a single mine in a small river’s watershed (1000 km2 watershed) has the potential to alter the fish community by decreasing the number or diversity of fishes. When considering the effect of mines (current or new), managers need to consider not only the local stream watershed but the downstream impact.

There is an opportunity for management and agencies to use the our results along with the advancements in GIS mapping data we have created to consider mine’s influence as a regional source of stress and improve fisheries through management actions. Mining will continue to be needed until an adequate substitution can be found. As a society, we should be thoughtful on where mines at placed, keeping them out of ecologically or culturally significant watersheds. Since, based on our results, a very low density of mines has the potential to alter the fish community in large areas.

We found strong associations between greater numbers of mines in watersheds and lower numbers and diversity of fishes. We tested both Even a single mine can damage fish habitats miles downstream - study (5)mineral and coal mines together and separately and saw associations with altered fish communities. We did not test specifically for mechanisms by which mines could affect stream fishes, but many other studies that have been conducted at smaller spatial scales have demonstrated specifically how mines can affect stream fishes (mines can be a source of sediments and chemicals into rivers, alter the flow of streams, and alter natural land covers all of which can change stream habitats). What makes our study unique is that it was conducted over a large spatial extent, and we repeated our analyses in each of three regions that cover all or portions of 33 states in the central and eastern US. Also, our associations were based on trends Even a single mine can damage fish habitats miles downstream - study (1)detected using 22,000 fish community samples.

Our results suggest that a single mine has potential to alter fish communities. We cannot provide a unique value for the distance the mines can influence fish communities. The distance downstream that mining can influence fish communities will vary based on stream size, number of mines, and regional variation in natural conditions. There is an opportunity for future studies to build upon our results and try to quantify and characterize distance downstream in various regions that mines influence aquatic communities.

The report, “Characterizing coal and mineral mines as a regional source of stress to stream fish assemblages,” was completed by Drs. Wesley Daniel, MSU associate professor of fisheries and wildlife Dana Infante, Robert Hughes at Amnis Opes Institute, Yin-Phan Tsang, Daniel Wieferich, Kyle Herreman, Arthur Cooper and William Taylor at MSU, Peter Esselman at the U.S. Geological Survey Great Lakes Science Center in Ann Arbor, Mich., and Lizhu Wang of the International Joint Commission Great Lakes Regional Office in Detroit,” was published in the journal Ecological Indicators, and was funded the US Fish and Wildlife and US Geological Survey.

Thinking of God leads to increased risk taking – study

Thinking of God leads to increased risk taking - study
Share this
Share

Leaps of faith? Gambling on Sundays may be more risky than churchgoers are aware, because, according to research by Radboud University, Netherlands scientists, activation of the concept of God in the minds of individuals increases their propensity to take risks.

Thinking of God leads to increased risk taking - study (3)
Dr Kai Qin Chan

“Risk taking is influenced by subtle environmental factors. It might not be a good idea to house a church service beside a casino, for example,” lead researcher Dr Kai Qin Chan of RU’s Department of Social and Cultural Psychology, told The Speaker.

Chan’s most recent research indicates that bringing the concept of God to the fore of people’s minds increases risk taking behavior, particularly when there is an incentive for gain.

The premise of the research was a hypothesis based on two fields of current research–recent psychological models that suggest religious belief provides a form of social control, and scientific findings that increased psychological control can lead to the formation fo riskier strategies. It made sense that these two fields of research could be brought together to show that religion could influence risk taking.

“We measured risk taking using a behavioral task,” Chan told us. “In this task, participants had to pump virtual balloons. With more ‘pumps’ they gave, the risk of explosion increases, but the chances of getting a larger reward increases as well, provided the balloon does not burst before they cashed in on their trial. We found that participants primed with God–for example, seeing the word ‘God’ briefly before doing the task–took more risk–they gave more pumps.”

All of the three studies conducted by the team showed that activating the God concept led to greater risk taking. The study participants were literally “taking a leap of faith,” according to the researchers.

However, this increased risk taking behavior was found to present only when participants felt they were in control of the situation.

“When we made one group of participants feel that they were not in control of things–i.e., we decreased their sense of psychological control–albeit momentarily–we found that these people look less risks, even when primed with God. This implies that priming with God (without any manipulation of psychological control) must have increased psychological control, because when we disrupted this process, risk taking returned to baseline levels.”

Chan’s research enabled him to make some educated guesses about the relationship between religion, morality and risk taking, but he was clear that questions of such relationships were difficult to answer, and that other great research was being done in those areas.

“I think much risk taking literature that examines how religion–e.g., religiosity–is related to lower risk taking may be an artifact of the measurement of risk taking,” said Chan.

“In these studies, sometimes investigators use measurements of risk taking that have an inherent moral component–for example, unprotected sex is risky, but it also has a tinge of one being morally loose. So, there are different domains of risk taking and we need to take them into account. Being risky in one domain does not necessarily translate into being risky in another, and because our notion of religion is so tightly linked with morality, risk-religion research needs to take into Thinking of God leads to increased risk taking - studyaccount the moral domains of risk taking as well.”

The research, the team found, contradicted certain other survey findings that religious people were less risk seeking than other people.

“Religion sometimes affects us in subtle ways,” observed Chan. “However, I do want to stress that I am not implying that religion is bad. Risk taking itself is not necessarily an evil, and I certainly do not want to say that religion makes people bad risk takers.”

The report, “Taking a Leap of Faith Reminders of God Lead to Greater Risk Taking,” was authored by Kai Qin Chan, Eddie Mun Wai Tong, and Yan Lin Tan of the National University of Singapore.

Small businesses aren’t hurt by giant competitors, sales are boosted if they can “stick it to the man”

Small businesses aren't hurt by giant competitors, sales are boosted if they can "stick it to the man"
Share this
Share

Small businesses that fear competing giant neighbors may need to think again. According to research by a joint team of university scientists, having a large competitor nearby may actually boost the sales of small businesses, but this depends on whether a small business can successfully “stick it to the man” in “framing the game.”

“When the owner of Los Angeles’s Coffee Bean & Tea Leaf could not stop Starbucks from moving in next door, he at first admitted defeat,” the team stated. “However, soon after, he was surprised to see his sales shoot up, so much so that he began to proactively locate new stores next to Starbucks.”

The research team, composed of Neeru Paharia of Georgetown University and Jill Avery and Anat Keinan of Harvard University, set out to test a theory that small businesses could benefit from big neighbors.

They examined shopping habits of two groups of participants. One group was told that a small bookstores only competitors were other small bookstores. Another group was told that the small bookstore was in competition with a nearby chain that threatened the future of the small bookstore.

The second group was more likely to buy at the small bookstore.

The team further explored the idea with a second study.

They gave participants a scenario: “Imagine you are in the mood for a cup of coffee. You can either go to Starbucks or an independent coffee shop called Joe’s Java.”

Again, there were two groups involved. The first was told that Joe’s Java and the chain were the same distance away, although in different directions. The second group learned that Joe’s and Starbucks were neighbors.

The second group–believing the two shops were neighbors–was the one that was more likely to patronize Joe’s Java.

The team believes that the results indicate an interest people may have in “sticking it to the man.”

This interest, the researchers argue, has something to do with the “framing-the-game effect.” Consumers, the researchers believe, want to be felt and heard in the marketplace, and do so through their purchase choices. Thus, they may feel motivated to exert their influence upon stores they wish to allow to succeed or fail.

The report, “Positioning Brands against Large Competitors to Increase Sales,” was authored by Neeru Paharia, Jill Avery, and Anat Keinan, and will be published in the upcoming in Journal of Marketing Research.

By Cheryl Bretton

Threatening children does not promote truth-telling – it can have the opposite effect

Threatening children does not promote truth-telling - it can have the opposite effect
Share this
Share

Threatening children does not promote truth-telling, according to research by McGill University scientists. In fact, using threats of punishment can have the opposite effect.

“Children often lie to try to avoid getting in trouble–especially when they have done something wrong,”  Victoria Talwar, professor of educational and counseling psychology at McGill University and lead researcher on the report, told The Speaker.

Victoria Talwar
Dr Victoria Talwar

“The bottom line is that punishment does not promote truth-telling,” Talwar said of her findings. “In fact, the threat of punishment can have the reverse effect by reducing the likelihood that children will tell the truth when encouraged to do so. This is useful information for all parents of young children and for the professionals like teachers who work with them and want to encourage young children to be honest.”

The study involved almost 400 children ages 4 through 8, who were each told not to peek at a toy while the researcher went out for a minute. Video cameras recorded that over two-thirds of the children peeked. Around the same amount of children lied about peeking.

For every month increase in age, children were less likely to peek, and for every month increase, children were more likely to lie and to be able to maintain their lies during later questioning.

“With age children have greater self-regulation/self control,” Talwar said. “We only left the room for a minute. If we had left for longer more children may have peeked. However, this is a common finding in the scientific literature that with age children are have better inhibitory control.”

The children more often told the truth when the experimenter told them that he or she would be happy if the child did so than if the experimenter told the children that the child him- or herself would be happy for telling the truth.

The research teams findings were what they expected–that the younger the child was, the more likely the reason they told the truth was to please an adult, but that older children more frequently told the truth because they felt it was the right thing to do.

Appeals based on punishment were not found to increase truth-telling. Overall, children were found to be less likely to tell the truth if they were afraid of being punished than either of the other two appeals.

Talway provided  some comments on alternatives to punishment that may be more likely to achieve the effect parents desire.

“What seems to increase honesty is giving children explicit messages about the value of honesty. If we wish to teach children to act in prosocial ways, we need to teach children about those behaviors and why they are important.”

“We need to teach children about the value of being honest,” Talway told us. “When a child does something wrong a natural reaction is to punish their transgression. However, if they tell the truth about it, we can give them some recognition for it. ‘I’m not happy you broke my vase–and you can help me clean it up/fix it/use your pocket money to replace it–but I’m glad you told me the truth.’ If we recognize honesty that is a powerful way to encourage and teach children that honesty is valued.”

The report, “The effects of punishment and appeals for honesty on children’s truth-telling behavior,” was authored by Victoria Talwar, Cindy Arruda, and Sarah Yachison, and was published in the Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

By Cheryl Bretton

 

How mothers treat their children has an effect on their adult romantic relationships–30 years later

How mothers treat their children has an effect on their adult romantic relationships--30 years later
Share this
Share

Sensitive early care–or the lack thereof–has a significant effect on the relationships of adults, according to new research by scientists at the University of Delaware and the University of Minnesota. The researchers found that early care had long-term implications for relationships with others–including intimate partners–and that the effects were seen across all racial, gender and socioeconomic lines. Sensitive care had clear implications even 30 years later.

How mothers treat their children has an effect on their adult romantic relationships--30 years later
Dr Lee Raby

“The total effect of early sensitivity for supportive parenting in adulthood was somewhat surprising to me considering that nearly three decades separated those two measurement time-points,” Dr Lee Raby, postdoctoral researcher at the Infant Caregiver Project at the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at University of Delaware, told The Speaker.

To see how people were affected by childhood experiences over the course of their lives, the team drew from an ongoing 37-year study that focused on maternal insensitivity.

“Our research report was based on data from the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation, which is a large research study of approximately 200 individuals who were born in the mid-1970’s to first-time mothers living in poverty,” Raby explained. “The participants have been continuously followed from birth to adulthood and currently are around age 37 years.”

The material included data on physiological responses, including skin conductance–an indicator of nervous system activity and therefore of emotional response.

The researchers wanted to find out if differences in the quality of care a child receives affected their nervous responses during adult conflicts.

How mothers treat their children has an effect on their adult romantic relationships--30 years laterThey found that children who received sensitive mothering were better able to deal with difficult relationship issues as adults–their skin reactivity measured relatively lower. Those children who received less sensitive care exhibited more nervous arousal as adults while attempting to deal with difficult situations, which means that those adults would tend more towards emotional avoidance and withdrawal.

The team controlled for other factors that could reasonably be expected to effect relationship abilities, and found that, at least for the study, the results were not dependent on the types of relationships to which an individual belonged–gender, ethnicity or socioeconomic status also were not responsible for the results.

“Specifically, we observed that early supportive/sensitive caregiving was associated with more competent functioning in (a) peer relationships during childhood and adolescence, (b) romantic relationships during young adulthood, and (c) parent-child relationships at age 32 years,” Raby told us.

“The findings related to peers and romantic partners are not new though,” he noted. “Previous publications from this research project have included those findings, and those associations have been replicated in independent samples.”

The conclusion reached by Raby and his team was that insensitive parenting created adults who avoided conflict with their spouses, and sensitive parenting led to an ability to resolve conflicts with romantic partners, but Raby added that further research into the matter was necessary.

“The association between early caregiving experiences and later parenting quality was largely indirect, meaning that more supportive early caregiving predicted later relationships with peers and romantic partners which in turn predicted adult parenting quality. However, in other studies we have observed that early caregiving experiences do have a unique role in promoting functioning in adult relationships even after accounting for relationship experiences during the intervening years. Because the unique contributions of early caregiving are relatively small, large samples and many measurements are needed to detect these associations. I anticipate that future research will provide evidence that early caregiving experiences shape later parenting outcomes both directly and indirectly.”

Why the type of treatment experienced by a child affects his or her later relationships may be due to the ideas that are formed by the child during early years.

“We think the reason for this is that individuals are developing expectations, attitudes, and behavioral skills within these earlier relationships that they carry into their interactions with their romantic partners. Bringing these two ideas together, the current study indicates that romantic relationship quality may be both a cause and a consequence of parenting quality,” said Raby, noting that more tightly controlled experimental studies are needed to more conclusively make these kind of causal claims.

We asked Raby if he would comment on the record levels of single parenthood in the US and the high incidence of broken families in many parts of the world–many of which face prolonged conflict–and if the research might have some implications on these large issues.

How mothers treat their children has an effect on their adult romantic relationships--30 years laterRaby qualified that he wanted to be cautious in how far he extended the findings, but offered some educated guesses on how the findings might relate.

“Our goal was to test theoretical ideas about the developmental origins of parenting behavior and the way it is transmitted across generations. That said, I do believe the findings speak to the larger issues.

“First, we observed—as have others—that the presence and quality of adults’ romantic partnerships predicted the quality of parenting they provided to their children. This is perhaps not surprising since romantic relationships are one of the biggest sources of social support as well as stress during adulthood. Second, we also observed that romantic relationship functioning has its origins, at least in part, in earlier experiences with caregivers and peers.

“In other words, individuals who experienced more supportive care from their parents early in life and were more skilled at interacting with peers during childhood and adolescence were more likely to form high-quality, committed romantic partnerships during adulthood.

“In my opinion, the important take-away message is also the most basic one,” said Raby. “Early parent-child relationships play an important role in understanding later parenting quality in adulthood. Although our study was focused on the interpersonal experiences that account for this intergenerational association, our findings in no way mitigate the importance of early caregiving experiences.

“Importantly, researchers working with other longitudinal studies have reported almost identical results concerning the intergenerational associations in parenting. This is astounding to me and makes me very excited about continuing to work on this topic.”

The report, “The Interpersonal Antecedents of Supportive Parenting: A Prospective, Longitudinal Study From Infancy to Adulthood,” was authored by Dr Lee Raby, Jamie Lawler, Rebecce Shlafer, Paloma Hesemeyer, Ander Collins and Alan Sroufe–researchers from both the University of Minnesota and the University of Delaware–and will be published in the journal Psychological Science the near future.

By Cheryl Bretton

Photos: Jessica Pankratz, Pete Labrozzi, United Nations

Thinspiration! Women don’t compare themselves with magazines–they live vicariously through them, study finds

Thinspiration! Women don't compare themselves with magazines--they live vicariously through them, study finds
Share this
Share

Women are not comparing themselves with the thin, attractive models in the magazines they enjoy–some, at least. Instead, women are living vicariously through the thin, attractive models–engaging in “thinspiration,” according to new research from Ohio State University. Not only that, women who enjoy these magazines are actually less likely to make an effort to look more attractive, the researchers found.

“Women get the message that they can look just like the models they see in the magazines, which is not helpful,” said researcher Silvia Knobloch-Westerwick, who is professor and graduate studies director at Ohio State University ‘s School of Communication. “It makes them feel better at first, but in the long run women are buying into these thinness fantasies that just won’t come true.”

The study involved 51 female college students who participated in an online test. The women evaluated magazine articles and advertisements dominated by featured thin-ideal images accompanied by text over the course of five days.

Knobloch-Westerwick examined the data for magazine reading habits, body mass index, body satisfaction, and especially tendency to compare their own form with that of others.

The options given to the participants included statements like, “This woman is thinner than me,” and, “I would like my body to look like this woman’s body.”

Results showed that women who compared themselves to the thin models had lower satisfaction with their own body by the end of the study. They were also more likely to have reported dieting during the period of the study.

Women who reported comparing their body and feeling that they would like to look more like the models, however, had increased body satisfaction by the end of the study. This phenomena Knobloch-Westerwick dubbed “thinspiration.”

Thinspiration is a concept in which people believe that they can make themselves as attractive as the models they view.

“They felt better about their body instantly when viewing the images and related content. They weren’t thinking about what they had to do to look like these models.

“These women felt better about their own bodies because they imagined that they could look just like the models they saw in the magazines.” The women who experienced the greatest “thinspiration” from looking at magazines were the least likely to engage in weigh-loss behaviour in the real world.

The research also found that over time the women began to identify with the models more.

“They may begin to feel affiliated with the models, and start to think this person is someone like me, someone I can be friends with and emulate,” she said.

Knobloch-Westerwick’s research was unlike many other body-image studies in that it found that viewing images of more ideal beauty models lead to higher body satisfaction. Knobloch-Westerwick said she suspected that because her images were accompanied by text–unlike the simple images of beauty used in most studies–participants were influenced by positive messages about how they could look like the models.

“If they just see an image of a thin model once and have to react immediately, they may indeed have poorer body satisfaction,” she said. “But if they look at images over the course of several days, readers may begin to feel more affiliated with the models, feel more like they could be like them. That could lead them to switch from comparing themselves negatively to the models to using these models as thinspiration.”

By Cheryl Bretton

Image: Kelly Povo